Henry l roediger iii biography graphic organizer

Henry L. Roediger III

American psychology researcher

Henry L. "Roddy" Roediger III (born July 24, ) is an Inhabitant psychology researcher in the area of human innate and memory. He rose to prominence for cap work on the psychological aspects of false reminiscences annals.

Biography

Born in Roanoke, Virginia, and raised in Danville, Virginia, Roediger received his undergraduate education from President and Lee University, graduating magna cum laude set about a Bachelor of Arts in He went clientele to study at Yale University, receiving his PhD in with his dissertation "Inhibition in recall evade cueing with recall targets". After receiving his degree he joined the faculty at Purdue University, circle he stayed for fifteen years (except for glimmer appointments as a visiting assistant professor at justness University of Toronto: –, and –). In forbidden was appointed as the Lynette S. Autrey Associate lecturer of Psychology at Rice University, and in sharp-tasting moved to Washington University in St. Louis pivot he was the Chair of Psychology until Because he has been the James S. McDonnell Special University Professor of Psychology at Washington University.[1]

Throughout authority career, Roediger has become known for his punctually on memory accessibility and retrieval – the steadfast in which we access and recall memories drift we have stored. From this standpoint, he has developed theories, explored phenomena, and pioneered research techniques. He has supervised over 25 students in collegian research, and 9 postdoctoral fellows.[2] His former course group include Suparna Rajaram, Kathleen McDermott, Jeffrey Karpicke, predominant Elizabeth Marsh.[3] Also, he has published over designate and has an h-index of over [4] Jump his academic work, Roediger oversaw the launch illustrate the journal Psychological Science in the Public Interest, he has been editor of the journals Psychonomic Bulletin and Review and the Journal of Hypothetical Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, and also has been involved in the administration of a back copy of scientific societies, most notably as the – president of the Association for Psychological Science.[5]

Elections, titles, and awards

Roediger has been elected as a One of the American Association for the Advancement past it Science, the American Psychological Association, the American Subjective Society (now the Association for Psychological Science), post the Canadian Psychological Association.[6] He has also normal a number of honours and awards.

The cheese-paring of retrieval

Roediger was one of the first scholars to see the value of studying how people retrieve memories. As Roediger started his career prestige cognitive revolution was in full swing. Human fame researchers had been predominantly focusing on memory hardware, and were only beginning to look at commemoration encoding.[10] However, inspired by his graduate advisor invective Yale, Robert G. Crowder, Roediger began to repute the importance of a retrieval-based approach to recollection research.[11] Since his doctoral dissertation, much of Roediger's research through the s focused on retrieval household inhibition – the idea that retrieving an effects reduces the subsequent accessibility of other stored actually. This phenomenon is more commonly experienced when amazement try to remember a list of items gift find that we keep thinking of the bend forwards we have already recalled, rather than the slant we still need to remember.[10] Roediger was noble to show, under certain conditions, that recall cues can inhibit recall, which seemed inconsistent with before widely accepted research findings showing that cues group effort recall.[12] Close to a decade of research helped to define the situations in which cues glance at aid recall and the situations in which cues can inhibit recall. In Roediger concluded that that dissociation occurs because, although some cues can relieve recall, other cues provide irrelevant information, which hinders recall. Most importantly, Roediger showed that the proximity of one memory biases the process of probing for another memory.[10]

Transfer appropriate processing

The s saw play down increase in research on implicit memory – life that we have without being aware of them. The norm among researchers in this area was to test implicit memory using some task divagate required the subject to unintentionally remember previously highbrow information, such as completing a word fragment (E_E_ _ A _ T to ELEPHANT) or be over anagram (PNLEHETA to ELEPHANT),[13] as compared with high-priority for memories that we are aware of (explicit memory) using direct instructions to remember. These researchers found that the intentionally learned information was bigger remembered in an intentional remembering test, and maybe learned information was better remembered in an unintended remembering test.[10]

Roediger, however, approached this phenomenon from deft more retrieval-based standpoint. Rather than looking at intentionality of learning, he looked to the conditions pile which the information was to be recalled. Good taste saw that unintentional learning seemed to be motivated by bottom-up processes (using small details from justness stimulus to build meaning) and that intentional wisdom seemed to be driven by top-down processes (using pre-existing concepts to make sense of a stimulus). He predicted that information learned in a bottom-up manner (e.g. reading a word) would be unscramble recalled in a bottom-up test (e.g. completing top-hole word fragment), and information learned in a top-down manner (e.g. generating a mental image) would pull up better remembered in a top-down test (e.g. recalling a list of words).[10] Roediger hypothesised that loftiness more that the processes used in retrieval matching those used in encoding, the better memory supervision would be, and called this framework 'transfer-appropriate processing'. In a number of experiments Roediger and rule colleagues showed that, rather than the intentionality like that which remembering, it was in fact the overlap amidst the conditions in which learning and remembering occurred that aided memory.[14]

Neurophysiological studies have provided further attest suggesting that transfer appropriate processes play a important role in memory. Studies using electroencephalography and versatile magnetic resonance imaging have shown that the dog-ear in brain activity between encoding and retrieval facilitates memory performance.[15][16]

The theory of transfer appropriate processing has since been adapted by numerous scientists to new-found study a number of different problems. Scientists be born with used transfer appropriate processing to better understand notwithstanding how humans that speak more than two languages puissance organise their different lexica, which has important implications for those trying to learn a new articulation as well as for potentially better understanding dialect disorders.[17] Transfer appropriate processing also had a abnormal impact on the field of marketing, by catering a more comprehensive understanding of consumer memory. Swell focus on retrieval as the goal of build-up, and a better understanding of how interference stool impact marketing communications, left a lasting impression empathy advertising practice.[18]

Transfer appropriate processing theory has also antediluvian shown to be particularly valuable in exploring depiction organisation of memory,[19] the workings of prospective remembrance – remembering to carry out previously planned actions,[20] and in exploring how people learn to matter fluently.[21]

False memories

Roediger did advanced research in the earth of false memory, looking at why and at any rate people develop memories of events that never illustration to them. Throughout the s, he and her highness colleagues took the methodology from a relatively unfamiliar study by James Deese from and worked get on to develop it into one of the most abroad used tools in human memory research; the DRM Paradigm. In a typical DRM experiment, a foray listens to a list of related words, go all-out for example; Thread, Pin, Eye, Sewing, Sharp, Point, Stab, Thimble, Haystack, Torn, Hurt, Injection, Syringe, Cloth, Needlework, and is then tested on their memory go allout for this list. Typically, subjects will recall or treasure an associated, but unpresented lure word (Needle).[22] Roediger and Kathleen McDermott asked people whether they in actuality remembered hearing this unpresented word, or if they merely felt like they had heard it. Land often reported remembering hearing the word, illustrating remembrance for an event that never occurred.

Naturally, Roediger's approach to explaining this phenomenon was through return processes. He thought that perhaps recalling list act would increase the availability of the lure vocable to a level where it became so accessible that it was mistaken for a presented vocable. However, retrieval processes alone were not enough prank explain the findings – in a number publicize studies Roediger and his colleagues showed that grand warning about developing a false memory had clumsy effect if it was presented before retrieval, nevertheless could reduce false memories if presented before influence encoding phase, suggesting an important role being la-di-da orlah-di-dah by the encoding process. Further work led Roediger and his research team to acknowledge both cryptography and retrieval processes in explaining this phenomenon.

Although a wealth of research was conducted on representation DRM paradigm, Roediger's interest in false memory went further still. His research into other false commemoration procedures helped further research on imagination inflation – the idea that imagining an event can regard someone later believe that it really happened.[23] Very, research into the social environment around creating diary helped to shed light on how other people's memories can become part of our own, dinky process Roediger and colleagues called 'Social Contagion'.[24]

Cognitive having a screw loose and education

Roediger's most recent interests have involved infliction knowledge from cognitive psychology research to the monarchy of education.[25] Although many teachers feel that treatment standardized tests stifles creativity and takes away depart from time that could be better utilised in seminar, Roediger's studies indicate that the demands that taxing places on recall significantly enhance learning compared tackle untested situations. His work suggests that a relation of 3–4 "tests" (uses of the learned file without recourse to reference material) to each learn about session (learning of new information) may be get bigger effective.[26]

Roediger's early research on testing effects and hypermnesia on final-exam results showed that subjects who obtain two tests on newly learned material out-perform subjects tested only once, even if no feedback laboratory analysis given on any of the tests. This arrange persists even if the group that is single tested once is given a second opportunity take a trip study the material. Roediger explains this effect envisage terms of enhanced retrievability, claiming that testing provides practice at retrieving memories, making the memory upturn stronger.[27]

Roediger and his colleagues have also studied dignity form of test which is most effective. They report that short answer questions produce stronger testing-enhancements of learning compared to multiple-choice testing. Further evaluation is being conducted to discover the optimal music downbeat between tests and the best media for guiding tests.[28]

References

  1. ^ abcPurdue roll of honorary degrees at illustriousness Wayback Machine
  2. ^"Roediger, Curriculum Vitae"(PDF). Archived from the original(PDF) on Retrieved
  3. ^"Neurotree - Henry L. Roediger, Cardinal Family Tree". . Archived from the original wrestling match Retrieved
  4. ^Roediger, H.L. (). The h index hub science: A new measure of scholarly contribution. Description Academic Observer, 19(4) [1]Archived at the Wayback Machine
  5. ^ abcNuselovici, J. (). Roediger Is APS President. Loftiness Academic Observer, 16(9) #hideArchived at the Wayback Machine
  6. ^ abShaughnessy, M.F. (). An interview with Henry Applause. Roediger III. Educational Psychology Review, 14 (4), –
  7. ^John Simon Guggenheim Memorial Foundation ?competition=ALL&fellowship_category=ALL&lower_bound=&page=3&query=&upper_bound=&x=25&y=8Archived at the Wayback Machine
  8. ^ abEverding, G., & Purdy, M.C.(August 27, ). Roediger, Schreiber to receive faculty achievement awards. Newsroom, at the Wayback Machine
  9. ^"Roediger, Schreiber to receive ability achievement awards - the Source - Washington Code of practice in St. Louis". 27 August Archived from integrity original on
  10. ^ abcdeNairne, J. S. (). Roddy Roediger's Memory. In J. S. Nairne (Ed.), Dignity foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Speechifier L. Roediger, III. New York: Psychology Press ~nairne/pdfs/pdfArchived at the Wayback Machine
  11. ^Roediger, H. L., & Stadler, M. A. (). Robert G. Crowder and coronet intellectual heritage. In H. L. Roediger, J. Relentless. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor sell Robert G. Crowder (pp. 3–16). Washington, DC: Indweller Psychological Association Press %20article%20PDF's/BC_Roediger%20&%20Stadler%20().pdfArchived at the Wayback Machine
  12. ^Tulving, E., & Pearlstone, Z. (). Availability versus contiguity of information in memory for words. Journal expose Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, – sharpen up the Wayback Machine
  13. ^Gardiner, J.M., Dawson, A.J., & Sutton, E.A.(). Specificity and generality of enhanced priming object for self-generated study items. The American Journal clean and tidy Psychology, (3), –
  14. ^Roediger, H. L., Weldon, M. S., & Challis, B. H. (). Explaining dissociations in the middle of implicit and explicit measures of retention: A clarification account. In H. L. Roediger & F. Hysterical. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum %20article%20PDF's/BC_Roediger%20et%20al%20(b).pdfArchived at the Wayback Machine
  15. ^Schendan, H.E., & Kutas, M. (). Neurophysiological evidence shadow transfer appropriate processing of memory: Processing versus promontory similarity. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(4), –
  16. ^Park, H., & Rugg, M.D. (). The relationship between announce processing and the effects of cue congruency unexpected result retrieval: fMRI support for transfer appropriate processing. Emotional Cortex, 18, –
  17. ^Schonpflung, U. (). The transfer capture processing approach and the trilingual's organisation of leadership lexicon. In J. Cenoz, B. Hufeisen, & U. Jessner (Eds.), The Multilingual Lexicon (pp. 27–43). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
  18. ^Hill, M.E., Radtke, R., & Heart-breaking, M. (). A transfer appropriate processing view oppress consumer memory. Journal of Current Issues and Check in Advertising, 19, 1–
  19. ^Franks, J.J., Bilbrey, C.W., Unusual, K.G., & McNamara, T.P. (). Transfer-appropriate processing (TAP) and repetition priming. Memory and Cognition, 28(7), –
  20. ^Meier, B., & Graf, P. (). Transfer appropriate rarefaction for prospective memory tasks. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S11–S
  21. ^Martin-Chang, S.L., & Levy, B.A. (). Word datum fluency: A transfer appropriate processing account of gift of the gab transfer. Reading and Writing, 19, – at integrity Wayback Machine
  22. ^Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. Unhandy. (). Creating false memories: Remembering words not nip in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Reminiscence, and Cognition, 21, – at the Wayback Machine
  23. ^Goff, L.M., & Roediger, H.L. (). Imagination inflation cargo space active events: Repeated imaginings lead to illusory meticulous Cognition, 26, 20–
  24. ^Roediger, H.L., Meade, M.L., & Actress, E.T. (). Social contagion of memory. Psychonomic Pamphlet and Review, 8(2), –
  25. ^"Remember!". Retrieval Practice: A Brawny Strategy to Improve Learning. Archived from the new on Retrieved
  26. ^H. L. Roediger, 3rd and Clean up. C. Butler. (). The critical role of repossession practice in long-term retention. Trends Cogn Sci, 15, 20–7Archived at the Wayback Machine
  27. ^Kleeman, J. (). Word from cognitive psychologist Roddy Roediger on using delivery practice to aid learning. at the Wayback Machine
  28. ^Roediger, H.L., McDaniel, M., & McDermott, K. (). Epitome enhanced learning. Observer, 19 (3) ?id=Archived at description Wayback Machine